• Refine Query
  • Source
  • Publication year
  • to
  • Language
  • 38
  • 19
  • 5
  • 3
  • 2
  • 1
  • 1
  • 1
  • Tagged with
  • 168
  • 168
  • 32
  • 30
  • 26
  • 19
  • 13
  • 13
  • 12
  • 12
  • 11
  • 11
  • 11
  • 10
  • 10
  • About
  • The Global ETD Search service is a free service for researchers to find electronic theses and dissertations. This service is provided by the Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations.
    Our metadata is collected from universities around the world. If you manage a university/consortium/country archive and want to be added, details can be found on the NDLTD website.
111

Epistémologie historique de l'étude du comportement animal

Bolduc, Jean-Sébastien 08 1900 (has links)
Projet réalisé dans le cadre d'une cotutelle avec l'Université de Bourgogne (Dijon, France) / Dans cette enquête nous entreprenons d’explorer la notion de comportement animal telle que l’exprime une discipline contemporaine, l’écologie comportementale. Afin de procéder à l’examen d’une notion aussi complexe, positionnée dans un contexte étroit, nous développons et utilisons un outil d’investigation : l’épistémologie historique. De façon générale, cet outil consiste à intégrer en une seule démarche les perspectives d’investigation diachronique et synchronique sur un même thème. Ainsi, pour procéder à l’examen de la notion de comportement animal, nous puisons d’abord dans l’histoire récente de l’écologie comportementale. Nous nous intéressons plus particulièrement à sa filiation avec l’éthologie classique et, après avoir reconstitué la trame historique qui unit les deux disciplines, nous procédons à leur comparaison. Cette seconde étape de notre épistémologie contribue à mettre en évidence plusieurs différences critiques dans la conception du comportement animal qu’endosse chacune des disciplines. Ces distinctions, en faisant ressortir la spécificité de l’écologie comportementale, nous permettent ensuite de nous intéresser à la notion de comportement animal à travers les approches principales que cette discipline mobilise. Ultimement, nous élaborons deux définitions de la notion de comportement animal. La première reflète le statut ontologique du comportement dans la discipline, alors que la seconde correspond à la conception qui se dégage de la pratique des écologues. / In this inquiry I undertake to explore the notion of animal behaviour as it is expounded in a contemporary field of inquiry: behavioural ecology. In order to carry out an analysis of such a complex notion, localized in a very narrow context, I design and use a specific tool of investigation called “historical epistemology”. Simply understood, this tool consists in the integration of diachronic and synchronic perspectives of investigation into a single approach to investigate a circumscribed theme. So, in order to proceed to the analysis of the notion of animal behaviour, I first draw into the recent history of behavioural ecology. I take special interest in its filiation with classical ethology and, after having reconstructed the historical frame that links the two fields together, I proceed to compare them. This comparison, the second step of my epistemology, is used to highlight the characteristics of the animal behaviour conceptions put forward by the two scientific disciplines. These distinctions, bringing to the fore the specificity of behavioural ecology, then allow me to scrutinize the notion of animal behaviour as it is instantiated in the main approaches mobilized by the discipline (especially what I identifies as the “phenotypic adaptationist”, the “phenotypic structural”, the “comparative” and the “by reduction” approaches). Last, I design two definitions of the notion of animal behaviour. The first one reflects the ontological status of the notion in this field of investigation, whereas the second corresponds to the conception underlying behavioural ecologist practices.
112

Developmental, morphological, and behavioural plasticity in the reproductive strategies of stink bugs and their egg parasitoids

Abram, Paul K 01 1900 (has links)
L’environnement façonne la physiologie, la morphologie et le comportement des organismes par l’entremise de processus écologiques et évolutifs complexes et multidimensionnels. Le succès reproducteur des animaux est déterminé par la valeur adaptative d’un phénotype dans un environnement en modification constante selon une échelle temporelle d’une à plusieurs générations. De plus, les phénotypes sont façonnés par l’environnement, ce qui entraine des modifications adaptatives des stratégies de reproduction tout en imposant des contraintes. Dans cette thèse, considérant des punaises et leurs parasitoïdes comme organismes modèles, j’ai investigué comment plusieurs types de plasticité peuvent interagir pour influencer la valeur adaptative, et comment la plasticité des stratégies de reproduction répond à plusieurs composantes des changements environnementaux (qualité de l’hôte, radiation ultraviolette, température, invasion biologique). Premièrement, j’ai comparé la réponse comportementale et de traits d’histoire de vie à la variation de taille corporelle chez le parasitoïde Telenomus podisi Ashmead (Hymenoptera : Platygastridae), démontrant que les normes de réaction des comportements étaient plus souvent positives que celles des traits d’histoires de vie. Ensuite, j’ai démontré que la punaise prédatrice Podisus maculiventris Say (Hemiptera : Pentatomidae) peut contrôler la couleur de ses œufs, et que la pigmentation des œufs protège les embryons du rayonnement ultraviolet; une composante d’une stratégie complexe de ponte qui a évoluée en réponse à une multitude de facteurs environnementaux. Puis, j’ai testé comment le stress thermique affectait la dynamique de la mémoire du parasitoïde Trissolcus basalis (Wollaston) (Hymenoptera : Platygastridae) lors de l’apprentissage de la fiabilité des traces chimiques laissées par son hôte. Ces expériences ont révélé que des températures hautes et basses prévenaient l’oubli, affectant ainsi l’allocation du temps passé par les parasitoïdes dans des agrégats d’hôtes contenant des traces chimiques. J’ai aussi développé un cadre théorique général pour classifier les effets de la température sur l’ensemble des aspects comportementaux des ectothermes, distinguant les contraintes des adaptations. Finalement, j’ai testé l’habileté d’un parasitoïde indigène (T. podisi) à exploiter les œufs d’un nouveau ravageur invasif en agriculture, Halyomorpha halys Stål (Hemiptera : Pentatomidae). Les résultats ont montré que T. podisi attaque les œufs de H. halys, mais qu’il ne peut s’y développer, indiquant que le ravageur invasif s’avère un « piège évolutif » pour ce parasitoïde. Cela pourrait indirectement bénéficier aux espèces indigènes de punaises en agissant comme un puits écologique de ressources (œufs) et de temps pour le parasitoïde. Ces résultats ont des implications importantes sur la réponse des insectes, incluant ceux impliqués dans les programmes de lutte biologique, face aux changements environnementaux. / The environment shapes the physiology, morphology, and behaviour of organisms through complex, multidimensional ecological and evolutionary processes. The reproductive success of individual animals is determined by how well their phenotype is suited to an environment that is constantly changing over single and multi-generational time scales. At the same time, phenotypes are shaped by the environment, which triggers adaptive modifications of animal reproductive strategies while also imposing important constraints. In this thesis, using stink bugs and their parasitoids as model organisms, I considered how several types of plasticity can interact to influence biological fitness, and how plasticity in reproductive strategies responds to several important components of environmental change (host quality, ultraviolet radiation, temperature, biological invasions). Firstly, I compared the response of behavioural and life history traits to body size variation in the parasitoid Telenomus podisi Ashmead (Hymenoptera: Platygastridae), finding that reaction norms of behavioural traits more often had positive slopes than life history traits. Next, I found that the predatory stink bug Podisus maculiventris Say (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) can selectively control the colouration of its eggs. Egg pigmentation in this species protects embryos against ultraviolet radiation as part of a complex oviposition strategy that evolved in response to a suite of environmental factors. Then, I tested how thermal stress affects the memory dynamics of the parasitoid Trissolcus basalis (Wollaston) (Hymenoptera: Platygastridae) learning the reliability of chemical traces left by its host. These experiments revealed that both high and low stressful temperatures prevented forgetting, affecting the time allocation of parasitoids on patches of host chemical traces. I also developed a general framework to classify temperature’s effects on all aspects of ectotherm behaviour, distinguishing constraints from adaptive behavioural adjustments. Finally, I tested the ability of an indigenous parasitoid (T. podisi) to attack the eggs of a new invasive pest of agriculture, Halyomorpha halys Stål (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae). The results showed that T. podisi attacks the eggs of H. halys but cannot develop, demonstrating that the invasive pest is an “evolutionary trap” for indigenous parasitoids, which could indirectly benefit native stink bug species by acting as an egg and time sink for the parasitoid. These findings have important implications for how insects, including those involved in biological control programs, respond to environmental change.
113

La perception du temps et sa modulation par la température chez les guêpes parasitoïdes

Parent, Jean-Philippe 04 1900 (has links)
Les modèles d'optimalité postulent que les animaux en quête de ressources utilisent le taux de gain de valeur adaptative pour optimiser plusieurs comportements tels que la répartition du temps lors de l’exploitation d‘un agrégat et l'investissement en progénitures. Bien que la durée de plusieurs comportements doit être régulée, peu d’évidences de la perception du temps sont actuellement disponibles pour les insectes et aucune pour les guêpes parasitoïdes, et ce malgré leur importance en tant que modèles écologiques. De plus, puisque les guêpes parasitoïdes sont poïkilothermes, cette capacité pourrait être affectée par la température. Nous avons supposé que les guêpes parasitoïdes auraient la capacité de percevoir le temps, à la fois de façon prospective (mesure du temps écoulé) et rétrospective (durée d'un événement passé), afin d'optimiser les décisions liées à l'exploitation d’agrégats d’hôtes et à la reproduction. Nous avons également émis l'hypothèse que la température aurait une incidence sur la perception du temps des guêpes parasitoïdes. Pour la mesure prospective du temps, nous avons utilisé la capacité d’apprentissage associatif de Microplitis croceipes (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). Les guêpes ont été entraînées à associer une odeur à la durée d'un intervalle entre des hôtes. Après leur entraînement, elles ont été testées dans un tunnel de vol avec un choix d’odeurs. Les guêpes ont choisi majoritairement l'odeur associée à l'intervalle de temps auquel elles étaient testées. Nous avons également investigué le rôle de la dépense énergétique sur la mesure du temps. Suite à une restriction de mouvement des guêpes pendant l'intervalle de temps entre les hôtes, elles choisissaient aléatoirement dans le tunnel de vol. L'absence de dépense énergétique les aurait rendues incapables de mesurer le temps. La dépense d'énergie est donc un substitut essentiel pour mesurer le temps. Pour la mesure rétrospective du temps, nous avons utilisé le processus d'évaluation de l'hôte de Trichogramma euproctidis (Hymenoptera: Trichogrammatidae). Certains trichogrammes utilisent la durée du transit initial sur l'œuf hôte afin d’en évaluer la taille et d’ajuster le nombre d’œufs à y pondre. Nous avons augmenté artificiellement la durée de transit initiale de T. euproctidis en suspendant l'œuf hôte pour le faire paraître plus gros qu'un œuf de taille similaire. Une augmentation de la durée de transit initiale a augmenté la taille de la ponte. Ceci démontre la capacité de T. euproctidis de mesurer la durée du transit initial, et donc d’une mesure du temps rétrospective. Pour déterminer si la température modifie la mesure du temps dans les espèces poïkilothermes, nous avons utilisé le comportement d’exploitation d’agrégats d’hôtes de T. euproctidis. Les modèles d’optimalités prédisent que les guêpes devraient rester plus longtemps et quitter à un faible taux de gain de valeur adaptative suite à un déplacement de longue durée plutôt que pour un déplacement de courte durée. Nous avons testé l'impact d'un déplacement de 24 h à différentes températures sur l'exploitation d’agrégats d’hôtes. Un déplacement à température chaude augmente le temps de résidence dans l’agrégat et diminue le taux de gain de valeur adaptative au moment de quitter ; ces comportements sont associés à un trajet de longue durée. L'inverse a été observé lors d’un déplacement à une température froide. Les températures chaude et froide ont modulé la mesure du temps en accélérant ou ralentissant l'horloge biologique, faisant paraître le déplacement respectivement plus long ou plus court qu’il ne l’était réellement. Ces résultats démontrent clairement que les guêpes parasitoïdes ont la capacité de mesurer le temps, autant rétrospectivement que prospectivement. Des preuves directes de leur capacité sont maintenant disponibles pour au moins deux espèces de guêpes parasitoïdes, une composante essentielle des modèles d'optimalité. Le rôle de la dépense énergétique dans la mesure du temps a aussi été démontré. Nos résultats fournissent également la preuve de l'impact de la température sur la perception du temps chez les insectes. L'utilisation de la dépense énergétique en tant que proxy pour mesurer le temps pourrait expliquer une partie de sa thermosensibilité, puisque les guêpes parasitoïdes sont poïkilothermes. Cette mesure du temps sensible à la température pourrait affecter des stratégies de lutte biologique. Sur le terrain, au début de la journée, la température de l'air sera similaire à la température de l'air autour des plantes infestées par des parasites, alors qu'elle sera plus chaude pendant la journée. En lutte biologique augmentative, les guêpes parasitoïdes libérées resteraient plus longtemps dans les agrégats d’hôtes que celles relâchées en début de journée. / Optimality models assume that animals foraging for resources use the rate of fitness gain to optimize several behaviours such as patch time allocation and progeny investment. Although the duration of multiple behaviors needs to be regulated, few evidence of time perception are currently available for insects and none for parasitic wasps, despite their importance as ecological models. This capacity to measure time could be affected by temperature, since parasitic wasps are poikilotherms. We hypothesized that parasitic wasps should have the capacity to perceive time, both prospectively (measure of elapsed time) and retrospectively (duration of a past event), in order to optimize foraging decisions related to host patch exploitation and reproduction. We also hypothesized that temperature would affect the time perception of parasitic wasps. For the prospective measure of time, we used the associative learning abilities of Microplitis croceipes (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). We trained them to associate an odour to the duration of an interval between hosts. Following their training, they were tested in a wind tunnel. The wasps chose significantly more the odour associated with the test interval they were trained to recognize. We also looked into the role of energy expenditure in the measure of time. When wasps were prevented from moving freely during the interval between hosts, they chose randomly in the wind tunnel. The lack of energy expenditure thus rendered them unable to measure time. Energy expenditure is therefore an essential proxy to measure time. For the retrospective measure of time, we used the host evaluation process of Trichogramma euproctidis (Hymenoptera: Trichogrammatidae). Trichogramma wasps use the duration of the initial transit on the host egg to evaluate its size and decide how many eggs to lay. We artificially increased the initial transit duration of T. euproctidis by suspending the host egg to make it appear larger than an unsuspended egg of the same size. An increase in initial transit duration increased clutch size in the host. This demonstrates the capacity of T. euproctidis to measure the duration of the initial transit duration, and therefore a retrospective measure of time. To ascertain if temperature modified the measure of time in poikilotherms species, we measured the patch exploitation behaviour of T. euproctidis following intervals at different temperatures. Wasps are expected to stay longer and leave at a lower rate of fitness gain following a long travel duration between host patches than for a short travel. We tested the impact of a 24 h travel duration at different temperatures on patch exploitation. An increase in temperature during travel time increased patch residence time and decreased the rate of fitness gain at departure, both of which are associated to an increase in travel time. The reverse was observed when travel occurred at a lower temperature. The increase or decrease in temperature affected the measure of time by speeding or slowing the clock respectively and making the travel appear longer or shorter than it actually was. Our results clearly show that parasitic wasps are able to measure time, both retrospectively and prospectively. There is now solid evidence that at least two species of parasitic wasps possess the capacity to measure time, an essential component of optimality models. We now also have evidence that energy expenditure is one of the proxies used to measure time. Our results are also the first evidence of the impact of temperature on time perception in insects. The use of energy expenditure as a proxy to measure time could explain some of its thermal-sensitivity, since parasitic wasps are poikilotherms. This temperature sensitive measure of time could affect biological control strategies. In augmentative biological control, releasing parasitic wasps during the day could lead to a higher level of patch exploitation than early in the day because of the temperature gradient between the air and the plants.
114

Fear in wildlife food webs: large carnivore predation risk mediates the impacts of a mammalian mesopredator

Suraci, Justin 27 April 2016 (has links)
Mounting evidence suggests that large carnivores regulate the abundance and diversity of species at multiple trophic levels through cascading top-down effects. The fear large carnivores inspire in their prey may be a critical component of these top-down effects, buffering lower trophic levels from overconsumption by suppressing large herbivore and mesopredator foraging. However, the evidence that the fear of large carnivores cascades through food webs has been repeatedly challenged because it remains experimentally untested. My collaborators and I exploited a natural experiment – the presence or absence of mesopredator raccoons (Procyon lotor) on islands in the Gulf Islands of British Columbia, Canada – to examine the breadth of mesopredator impacts in a system from which all native large carnivores have been extirpated. By comparing prey abundance on islands with and without raccoons, we found significant negative effects of raccoon presence on terrestrial (songbirds and corvids), intertidal (crabs and fish) and shallow subtidal (red rock crabs Cancer productus) prey, demonstrating that, in the absence of native large carnivores, mesopredator impacts on islands can extend across ecosystem boundaries to affect both terrestrial and marine communities. To test whether fear of large carnivores can mitigate these community-level impacts of mesopredators, we experimentally manipulated fear in free-living raccoon populations using month-long playbacks of large carnivore vocalizations and monitored the effects on raccoon behaviour and the intertidal community. Fear of large carnivores reduced raccoon foraging to the benefit of the raccoon’s prey, which in turn affected a competitor and prey of the raccoon’s prey. By experimentally restoring the fear of large carnivores in our study system, we succeeded in reversing the impacts of raccoons, reinforcing the need to protect large carnivores given the conservation benefits the fear of them provides. Our experimental work demonstrated that fine-scale behavioural changes in prey in response to predation risk can have community-level effects relevant to biodiversity conservation. However, experimentally testing animal responses to predators and other sources of risk in free-living wildlife presents considerable logistical challenges. To address these challenges, my collaborators and I developed an Automated Behavioural Response system, which integrates playback experiments into camera trap studies, allowing researchers to collect experimental data from wildlife populations without requiring the presence of an observer. Here I describe tests of this system in Uganda, Canada and the USA, and discuss novel research opportunities in ecology and conservation biology made available by this new technology. / Graduate
115

Analyse der Futter- und Wasseraufnahme sowie von Merkmalen des Aktivitätsverhaltens bei Milchkühen im ante partalen Zeitraum

Raya, Siraj 08 August 2011 (has links)
In Untersuchungen an 76 hochtragenden Friesian Milchkühen wurden durch sensorgestützte Messverfahren die Parameter des Fress-, Wasseraufnahme-, Bewegungs- und Liegeverhaltens erfasst. Ziel der vorliegenden Arbeit ist die Analyse und Modellierung ausgewählter Verhaltensparameter von Hochleistungskühen im antepartalen Zeitraum mittels automatischer Überwachungssysteme. Ein weiteres Ziel war die Bewertung der Verhaltensparameter in Bezug auf ihre Aussagequalität bei der Erkennung von Abkalbungen und Geburtsverlauf. Die Studie zeigt, dass die Merkmale des Tierverhaltens der Milchkühe kurz vor der Geburt geändert wurden. In Kombination mit geeigneten Auswertungsmethoden können Parameter des sensor-gestützten Tiermonitoring genutzt werden, um den Zeitpunkt und den Verlauf der Abkalbung vorhersagen zu können. Mit Hilfe einer Strukturbruchanalyse wurde 24 Stunden vor der Abkalbung ein deutliches Signal registriert, für alle Tiere zutreffend und unabhängig vom Geburtsverlauf , das geeignet ist, die Vorhersage des Geburtszeitpunktes vorzunehmen. Für die letzten 24 Stunden a.p. konnten für Kühe mit leichtem oder schwerem Geburtsverlauf sehr unterschiedliche Konstanten und Koeffizienten des Gleichungssystems berechnet werden. Es ist festzuhalten, dass ausgewählte Parameter des sensorgestützten Tiermonitoring in Kombination mit den zur Anwendung gelangten Auswertungsmethoden (Strukturbruchanalyse) geeignet sind, sowohl den Zeitpunkt als auch den Grad des Abkalbeverlaufs vorhersagen zu können. / This study was carried out to monitor the behaviour in the prepartum period of 76 Holstein dairy cows in the last week of pregnancy by using a sensor-based technology. The reviewed behaviour indicators were lying behaviour and moving activity, Dry matter intake, Feeding behaviour, Water intake and Drinking Behaviour. Cows showed differences in their behaviour especially in the last 24 hours before calving. The Change-Point Analysis, Chow-Test and CUSUMQ-Test method proofed that there is a significant difference in the behaviour between 72 h. to 24 h. a.p. and 24 h. to the time of calving. It can be assumed that a sensor-based technique combined with different data analyzing methods can be used to predict the time of calving and the calving ease.
116

Aspectos neuroimunes de camundongos tratados com morfina. / Neuroimmune aspects of morphine-treated mice.

Costa, Elaine Cristina Rodrigues da 17 June 2010 (has links)
Injeções repetidas de psicoestimulantes e/ou compostos opióides desencadeiam respostas comportamentais, bioquímicas, endócrinas e celulares diferentes daquelas induzidas pela exposição aguda. Em particular, a morfina, considerada como o protótipo do estimulante opioidérgico, tem despertado grande interesse devido a seus efeitos múltiplos. Assim, no presente estudo avaliaram-se as consequências dos tratamentos agudo e repetido com morfina (20mg/kg) em camundongos sobre os seguintes aspectos: atividade geral; níveis séricos de corticosterona; concentrações corticais, estriatais e hipotalâmicas de noradrenalina, dopamina e serotonina, de seus metabólitos e as taxas de renovação destes neurotransmissores; atividade funcional ex vivo de neutrófilos sanguíneos e de macrófagos peritoneais, sendo estes desafios realizados in vivo com diferentes estímulos imunológicos, infecciosos ou não; crescimento tumoral e a sobrevida de camundongos portadores de um tumor ascítico de Ehrlich. Os resultados obtidos mostraram que os tratamentos agudo e repetido com morfina modulam diferentemente as repostas comportamental e neuroendócrina, dependendo do tempo de observação pós-desafio. Portanto, conclui-se que camundongos tratados repetidamente com morfina (20mg/kg) apresentam alterações comportamentais e neuroendócrinas que, no entanto, não foram acompanhadas por diferenças nas avaliações de atividade imune inata. / Repeated administrations of psychostimulant and/or opioid compounds trigger different behavioral, biochemical, endocrine and cellular responses as compared with those induced by acute exposure. Morphine, has attracted great interest due to its multiple effects. Thus, this study evaluated the effects of acute and repeated treatment with morphine (20mg/kg) in mice on the following: locomotor activity; serum levels of corticosterone; cortical, striatal and hypothalamic concentration of dopamine, noradrenaline and serotonin, as well as their metabolites and turnover; blood neutrophils and peritoneal macrophage activity ex vivo, which were challenged with different immunological stimulants; tumor growth and survival of mice with Erlich ascitic tumor. The results showed that acute and repeated morphine treatment differently modulated behavioral and neuroendocrine responses, depending on the these period after the injection challenge. Thus, it is concluded that mice repeatedly treated with morphine (20mg/kg) present behavioral and neuroendocrine changes; however, this changes were not accompanied by variation in innate immune activity.
117

Modificação na estrutua do pasto e no comportamento ingestivo de bovinos durante o rebaixamento do capim-marandu submetido a estratégias de pastejo rotacionado / Modifications in sward structure and ingestive behaviour of cattle during the grazing down process of marandu palisadegrass subjected to rotational grazing strategies

Trindade, Júlio Kuhn da 07 March 2007 (has links)
Práticas de manejo afetam a estrutura do dossel forrageiro, podendo afetar os padrões de deslocamento, procura e ingestão de forragem pelos animais em pastejo. O objetivo deste trabalho foi avaliar o efeito de estratégias de pastejo rotacionado sobre o comportamento animal e padrões de ingestão de forragem de bovinos de corte em pastos de Brachiaria brizantha cv. Marandu. O experimento foi realizado no Departamento de Zootecnia da USP/ESALQ, de novembro de 2005 a abril de 2006. Os tratamentos experimentais compreenderam a combinação entre duas intensidades (altura pós-pastejo de 10 e 15 cm) e dois intervalos entre pastejos (período de tempo necessário para se atingir 95 e 100% de interceptação luminosa pelo dossel durante a rebrotação - IL), e foram alocados às unidades experimentais (piquetes de 1.200 m²) segundo um delineamento inteiramente casualizado e arranjo fatorial 2 x 2, com 3 repetições. As avaliações foram realizadas de forma repetida em duas épocas do ano: (1) primavera (novembro e dezembro de 2005), e (2) verão (janeiro a abril de 2006). A densidade de lotação foi dimensionada para que a duração do período de ocupação fosse de 10 a 12 horas. Foram avaliadas as seguintes variáveis-resposta: distribuição vertical dos componentes morfológicos da massa de forragem, dinâmica do rebaixamento dos pastos, padrões de ingestão (massa do bocado, taxa de bocados e taxa de consumo), comportamento (pastejo, ruminação e outras atividades), composição morfológica da forragem consumida; padrões de deslocamento e busca por alimento (número de estações alimentares por unidade de tempo e número de passos entre estações alimentares), e padrões de desfolhação de perfilhos individuais (profundidade de desfolhação e freqüência e intensidade de desfolhação de folhas em geral e nas categorias folhas em expansão e expandidas). As maiores taxas de rebaixamento foram registradas para os tratamentos 100/10 e 100/15, enquanto as menores para o tratamento 95/15. De modo geral, à medida que os animais permaneceram nos piquetes e ocorria depleção do estrato pastejável, a taxa de rebaixamento diminuiu em virtude da diminuição da altura do dossel e do aumento da presença de colmos e de material morto no horizonte de pastejo. Apesar de a intensidade de pastejo menos severa (altura pós-pastejo de 15 cm) resultar, no início do pastejo, em menor bocado, taxa de consumo e intensidade de desfolhação de folhas em expansão e expandidas, os animais acabaram consumindo uma forragem com maior proporção de folhas. Ao longo do rebaixamento, os tratamentos de 100% IL resultaram em bocados maiores, mas, no entanto, a taxa de consumo e a proporção de folhas na forragem consumida foram menores em relação aos tratamentos de 95% de IL. Os tratamentos 95/10 e 95/15 estiveram associados com maior atividade de pastejo e maior taxa de consumo, sugerindo maior consumo diário. Entretanto, o tratamento 95/15 foi o que resultou na maior proporção de folhas na forragem consumida, e foi o tratamento em que a intensidade e freqüência de desfolhação de folhas foram mais baixas, condicionadas pelas menores densidades de lotação empregadas. Esse padrão de desfolhação resultou em uma maior área foliar remanescente, favorecendo a rebrota seguinte e o rápido retorno dos pastos à utilização. A estratégia de pastejo que resultou em melhor utilização da forragem produzida, foi aquela em que os pastejos foram realizados com 95% de IL até uma altura pós-pastejo de 15 cm. / Management practices affect sward structure and may affect patterns of animal movement, search and forage ingestion during grazing. The objective of this study was to evaluate the effect of rotational grazing practices on animal behaviour and patterns of forage ingestion of beef cattle on Brachiaria brizantha cv. Marandu. The experiment was carried out at Departamento de Zootecnia, USP/ESALQ, from November 2005 to April 2006. Treatments corresponded to combinations between two grazing intensities (post-grazing residues of 10 and 15 cm) and two grazing frequencies (equivalent to the period of time necessary for swards to reach 95 and 100% interception of the incident light during regrowth ? LI), and were allocated to experimental units (1200 m2 paddocks) according to a completely randomised design and a 2 x 2 factorial arrangement, with 3 replications. Measurements were performed in two seasons of the year: (1) spring (November and December 2005), and (2) summer (January to April 2006). Stocking density was calculated aiming at an occupation period of paddocks of 10 to 12 hours. The following response variables were analysed: vertical distribution of morphological components of sward herbage mass, dynamics of the grazing down process, patterns of ingestion (bite mass, bite rate, and intake rate), animal behaviour (grazing, rumination and other activities), morphological composition of the consumed herbage, patterns of animal movement and search for forage (number of feeding stations per unit of time and number of steps between feeding stations), and patterns of defoliation of individual tillers (defoliation depth, frequency and intensity of defoliation of leaves ? expanding and expanded leaves). The highest rates of decrease in sward height were recorded for treatments 100/10 and 100/15, and the lowest for treatment 95/15. In general, as the occupation period progressed, the grazing stratum was depleted and there was a decrease in the rate swards were lowered, a result of lower sward heights and increased presence of stems and dead material in the stratum used by the animals for grazing. In spite of the lower grazing intensity (post-grazing height of 15 cm) has resulted, at the beginning of grazing, in smaller bites, intake rate and defoliation intensity of expanding and expanded leaves, the ingested herbage had a higher proportion of leaves compared to grazings at 10 cm residue. During the grazing down process, the 100% LI treatments resulted in heavier bites, but lower intake rate and proportion of leaves in the herbage consumed than the 95% LI treatments. Treatments 95/10 and 95/15 were associated with the largest grazing activity and highest intake rate, suggesting higher daily intake. However, 95/15 was the treatment that resulted in the largest proportion of leaves in the herbage consumed, and in the lowest values of frequency and intensity of defoliation of leaves, due to the lowest stocking density used. This pattern of defoliation resulted in a high residual leaf area after grazing, favouring regrowth and quick return of swards to grazing. The grazing strategy that allowed more efficient harvest of herbage was that where grazings were initiated with 95% LI and finished with a 15 cm post-grazing height.
118

Behavioural circadian rhythms : a novel approach to monitor sheep in extensive systems and study the differences between beef steers for methane emission, feed efficiency and growth / Ritmos circadianos comportamentais : um novo modo de monitorar ovinos em sistemas extensivos e estudar a emissão de metano, eficiência alimentar e crescimento de bovinos de corte

Sarout, Bruna Nunes Marsiglio January 2017 (has links)
As tecnologias baseadas em sensores estão cada vez mais disponíveis e podem ser usadas para coletar informações detalhadas sobre o comportamento animal. Com esta informação é possível avaliar o ritmo circadiano de variáveis comportamentais e monitorar sua resposta. A identificação de variações na resposta deste ritmo tem o potencial de detectar problemas de saúde e questões de bem-estar animal. O objetivo deste trabalho foi estudar os ritmos circadianos comportamentais como uma nova abordagem para monitorar ovelhas em sistemas extensivos e estudar a emissão de metano, eficiência alimentar e crescimento de novilhos de corte. Este trabalho foi composto por dois experimentos (ovinos e bovinos). Foram utilizados cochos automatizados e sensores de atividade baseados em acelerômetro para coletar informações detalhadas do comportamento ingestivo (bovinos) e do comportamento de atividade (ovinos e bovinos), juntamente com as características de desempenho animal. Estes dados foram utilizados para calcular a percentagem de comportamento cíclico harmônico/sincronizado a cada período de 24 h. Essa porcentagem é chamada de grau de acoplamento funcional (DFC) e é calculada com uso de um período móvel de sete dias. No experimento com ovinos, um total de 29 ovelhas Scottish Blackface foram monitoradas por quatro semanas em cada estação do ano, em sistema extensivo nas terras altas da Escócia. Dados meteorológicos foram coletados diariamente. Modelos estatísticos de regressão com efeito aleatório foram utilizados para avaliar a variação da resposta entre indivíduos. Houve uma forte dinâmica criada pelas estações do ano e pelo ciclo produtivo/fisiológico das ovelhas. Durante a primavera e o verão, o desvio padrão do DFC foi um melhor estimador do ganho de peso quando comparado ao índice de moção. A combinação da análise do DFC e o agrupamento de indivíduos com base em sua resposta às variáveis ambientais oferece potencial para obter informações relevantes para o manejo do rebanho. O experimento de bovinos foi conduzido com duas dietas contrastantes (volumoso: concentrado 8:92 e 50:50) e duas raças (40 mestiços Charolês e 40 Luing). Os padrões diurnos de ingestão e atividade foram altamente sincronizados. O ritmo circadiano da atividade foi importante para explicar as diferentes emissões de metano entre indivíduos, independente da raça ou dieta, e também teve ligação com a eficiência alimentar e o crescimento dos novilhos. Este trabalho mostra a importância dos ritmos circadianos comportamentais e como essas abordagens podem melhorar a qualidade e o significado dos dados provenientes de sensores automatizados. / Sensor-based technologies are becoming increasingly available and can be used to gather detailed information about animal behaviour. With this information it is possible to assess animal behavioural circadian rhythm and monitor its response. Identifying breakdowns of this rhythm has the potential to detect health problems and animal welfare issues. The aim of this work was to study the behavioural circadian rhythms as a novel approach to monitor sheep in extensive systems and to study the differences between beef steers production traits, in methane emission, feed efficiency and growth. This work consisted of two experiments, one dealing with sheep in an extensive system and the other with housed beef steers. Automated feed intake equipment and accelerometer-based activity sensors were used to collect detailed information on feed intake (for cattle) and activity behaviour (for sheep and cattle), alongside animal performance characteristics. These data were used to calculate the percentage of cyclic behaviour that is harmonic/synchronized to each 24 h period as Degree of Functional Coupling (DFC) shown within rolling seven day periods. In the sheep experiment, in total twenty-nine Scottish Blackface ewes were monitored for four consecutive weeks in each season across a full year, in an extensive system on Scottish upland pastures. Weather data were collected daily. Random regression statistical models were used to assess between-individual variation in response to the weather. There was a strong dynamic created by the seasons and by the production and physiological cycle in sheep in these high latitude systems. Over the spring and summer period, the variation in the response of DFC was a better estimator of BWG (Body Weight Gain) than the use of a simple motion index. The combination of circadian rhythm analysis and the clustering of individuals into groups based around their regression response to environmental variables provides considerable potential to glean information relevant for group and individual animal management. The cattle experiment was conducted with two contrasting diets (concentrate-based and mixed diet) and two breeds (40 crossbred Charolais and 40 purebred Luing). The diurnal patterns of feeding and activity behaviours were strong and highly synchronised. Activity rhythmicity was well suited to show up differences between individual methane emissions independent of breed or diet, and it was also well related to important production traits as feed efficiency and growth of beef steers. This work shows the importance of the behavioural circadian rhythms and that these approaches may enhance the quality and meaningfulness of data coming from automated sensors.
119

Peak Shift in Remembering

Hoan, Andros January 2003 (has links)
If remembering is discriminative behaviour along the dimension of time and if, as Sargisson and White (2001) argued, generalisation around a peak can occur in this behaviour, then the peak shift which has been shown in discrimination along so many other stimulus dimensions, might also occur in remembering. To examine this hypothesis, 6 hens were trained in a delayed matching-to-sample procedure at delays of 2 and 4 s. The probability of reinforcement for correct responses was initially 0.9 at both delays until performance stabilised. A generalisation probe was then carried out by inserting unreinforced trials at delays of 0, 1, 1.5, 2.5, 3, 3.5, 4.5, 5 and 6 s in a session amongst normal training delay trials. The generalisation functions had a slight peak around 2 s. After further training, a second generalisation probe showed a slightly declining function. The probability of reinforcement at the 2 s delay was then dropped to 0.1, so that in the terms of the classical generalisation/peak shift paradigm, 2-s delay trials became S¯ and 4-s delay trials became S+. A third generalisation probe then was conducted. This resulted in a flat function from 0 s to 3 s, and a large, clear peak in discriminative performance at 4.5 s over all hens. After more of the same differential reinforcement training, a fourth generalisation probe showed a broad curve peaking at 3 s, with minima at 1 s and 6 s and a global maximum at 0 s. Another training condition was then run, with the probability of reinforcement at the 2-s delay dropped to 0, to see if increasing the aversiveness of S¯ would again result in a peak shift. A fifth generalisation probe was then conducted. This showed a sharp decline in discriminability at shorter delays, a dip around 2 s, and a very small area shift beyond 4 s, but no clear peak shift. This was interpreted as being due to overlearning, with the consequences of remembering at S¯ no longer significantly affecting performance at S+. A final training condition was then run, with S¯ moved from 2 s to 3 s with zero probability of reinforcement, and for only a short period, to prevent overlearning. It was predicted that this would cause peak shift to re-occur. A sixth generalisation probe was then conducted. This found a further decline in discriminability at shorter delays, a shift in the dip from 2 s to 3 s, and a large, clear peak at 4.5 s. This demonstration of peak shift in a remembering process would not have been predicted by any traditional theory of memory, but strongly supports the conception of remembering as discriminative behaviour along the stimulus dimension of time.
120

Abundance, behaviour and habitat use patterns of Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (<em>Tursiops aduncus</em>) in the Clarence and Richmond River estuaries in northern New South Wales, Australia

Fury, Christine Ann Unknown Date (has links)
Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops spp.) are a widely studied species in marine habitats, however, information on estuarine populations in Australia is very limited. To fully understand the importance of estuaries as habitats for dolphins there needs to be clear quantitative data on dolphin populations and their habitat use in estuaries. This study provides the first published data on Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) abundance estimates, site fidelity, individual ranging patterns, habitat use, flood impacts and sexual segregation patterns over a 3- year period in the Clarence River (CR) and Richmond River (RR) estuaries in northern New South Wales, Australia.The results indicate that, at present, the CR estuary is sustaining a larger dolphin community with a predominantly resident population compared to the RR estuary, which supports a smaller community with lower site fidelity. The CR estuary dolphin abundance estimate of 71 (62-81 95% CI, CV = 0.07) is more than twice the size of the RR estuary abundance estimate of 34 (19- 49 95% CI, CV = 0.23). Differences in site fidelity were observed between the estuaries with 60% and 37% of identified dolphins determined as residents, 26% and 21% as occasional visitors, and 14% and 42% as transients for the CR and RR, respectively. Resource partitioning was apparent in both estuaries with the mean distance resident dolphins were found upstream from the River mouth being greater than for the occasional visitors and transients.Tursiops aduncus was seen all year round in the CR and RR estuaries, with peak sightings occurring in spring at both sites. In the CR the dolphin population showed consistent seasonal fluctuations, whereas this did not occur in the RR population. In the CR the largest spatial distribution of dolphins in the estuary was observed in spring and winter, while in summer they were primarily restricted to the main estuary channel. Different behaviours that were observed; feeding, socialising, travelling, and milling and resting, were found to be influenced by season, tidal phase and tidal range. In both estuaries the core habitat areas used by the population for feeding consisted of areas with considerable slope near the edge of tidal sand banks, adjacent to deeper channels at the entrances of canals, creeks or artificial breakwalls. In addition, the core habitat areas used by the population for milling and resting behaviour in both estuaries occurred in shallow, sheltered areas, often associated with seagrass beds. Socialising occurred more frequently in the CR throughout most of the deeper waters of the estuary, whereas in the RR it was primarily restricted to a small area of medium depth in the estuary.The major determinant of T. aduncus occupancy in the two estuaries was the flood events that occurred, which resulted in the dolphins abandoning the estuary. The mean predicted probabilities for sighting dolphins during non-flood periods were 0.87 and 0.71, during a flood 0.21 and 0.04, and during a post-flood recovery period 0.83 and 0.80 in the CR and RR, respectively. Principal Components Analysis (PCA) showed that when the dolphins were absent from the estuaries, three components were extracted from the water quality parameters in the CR, and two components in the RR. High loadings from the PCA were associated with the changing salinity, turbidity, pH, dissolved oxygen and temperature associated with the flood events. The return of the dolphins to the estuary following a flood depended on the length and severity of the flood event, but generally the dolphins seemed to prefer waters with salinity levels above 29 ‰. This could be associated with higher salinities being important for their physiological health, or because their prey returned to the estuaries during these higher salinity conditions, more likely a combination of both of these factors.Sexual segregation patterns were observed in T. aduncus populations with mixed gender and female groups mainly recorded in the CR, whereas in the RR female groups dominated the estuary. In the CR, significant differences occurred in sightings between the mixed and female groups in relation to water depth and behaviour. Mixed gender groups were sighted predominantly in deeper water and were involved in social behaviour including sexual behaviour and male herding of females. In contrast, the female groups were observed across all water depths, predominately feeding and also participating in more milling and resting behaviours.The high occurrence of aggressive herding behaviour by males in the CR was significantly different at varying depths, tides and seasons, occurring more often in deeper water, at higher tides and in non-breeding seasons. Female groups were found to utilise the small, shallow tributaries and travel for longer distances up these smaller tributaries than the mixed groups, which were concentrated in the deeper main channel of the estuary.The deeper water of the channels may facilitate the males in herding the females, while the female groups’ habitat selection of shallow estuary areas may provide a sanctuary from aggressive males, access to suitable prey items or prey density for mothers and their calves, or a combination of these factors.This study has provided the first detailed research on T. aduncus dolphin population dynamics, habitat use, occupancy and sexual segregation patterns in two Australian subtropical estuaries. To ensure the long-term survival of both of these dolphin populations, management of future increased anthropogenic disturbances from boat traffic, pollution, dolphin watching, industrial or urban development, over-fishing and habitat degradation of the catchment is needed. Good quality water conditions, the protection of the core feeding areas and small shallow tributaries for females and their calves, and sheltered areas for resting behaviour all need to be maintained for the continued conservation of these important dolphin populations.

Page generated in 0.0764 seconds